Guest guest Posted April 28, 2009 Report Share Posted April 28, 2009 Here seems to be a counter attack to poo-poo people with ES. " Rubin has little doubt that the symptoms exhibited by electrosensitives are real. “Something cognitive is going on – something to do with the expectation is triggering the symptoms,” he says. And the idea of a psychological basis to electrosensitivity is supported by the fact that cognitive behavioural therapy can be an effective treatment. " Any body know of an effective " cog behav therapy? " Full Transcript: Why health warnings can be bad By Stuart Blackman Published: April 25 2009 01:27 | Last updated: April 28 2009 12:33 http://www.ft.com/cms/s/2/0d9499ec-2d75-11de-9eba -00144feabdc0,dwp_uuid=a712eb94-dc2b-11da-890d-0000779e2340.html It is perhaps surprising that scientists behind an experiment conducted at the University of California 25 years ago were able to recruit any volunteers. The researchers, purporting to study the effects of electric currents on brain function, warned potential participants that electrodes would be strapped to their temples, and could cause serious headaches. Despite this, 34 students came forward. At the end of the trial, two thirds of the volunteers did report headaches – despite the electric current never having been turned on. Such is the power of the “nocebo” effect, the real subject of the experiment. The current had not been switched on because the scientists were investigating whether expectation alone could make healthy people ill. Apparently it could. In the 25 years since then, research has been accumulating that the nocebo effect – the evil twin of the better-known placebo effect – is a widespread phenomenon that affects many aspects of our lives and which might contribute to a host of 21st-century ailments, from food allergies to obesity, chronic fatigue, back pain and electrosensitivity. Some experts are concerned that the situation is being compounded by the efforts of health professionals to raise awareness of threats. “When someone receives a placebo and they get better, it’s because of a variety of self-healing processes in the body that are cognition-related,” says , a psychologist at the National University of Ireland, Galway, who specialises in how mental stress affects physical health. “One expects to get healed and certain symptoms go away.” In the case of the nocebo, it is negative expectations that become self-fulfilling prophecies. When we expect something to make us ill – electrodes wired to our temples, for example, or, more routinely, a medicine with known side-effects – we start looking for signs of illness. And we’ll probably find some, says , even if the pill is a dummy one or the electric field a sham. That is because unpleasant physical symptoms are a normal part of life for perfectly healthy people. Headaches come and go. Some nights it is hard to get much sleep, and some days it is difficult to keep our eyes open. We might feel light-headed one moment and in a bad mood another. These are all experiences that we would not think twice about were we not looking for signs that things are wrong. But when we are looking, it is easy to interpret a bad night’s sleep as insomnia, tiredness as fatigue, light-headedness as dizzy spells or a bad mood as depression – and then to reattribute those symptoms to whatever it was that we expected to harm us. And once we start believing that something is making us ill, we get anxious, which can itself exacerbate existing symptoms or induce others. “Anxiety generally leads to elevations in blood pressure and immune deficiency,” says . And more symptoms mean more anxiety. Everyday expressions such as “worried sick” or “scared to death” suggest that the link between negative emotions and ill-health was made long before the phenomenon was given a medical label. But once in the realm of scientific investigation, the nocebo effect started turning up with surprising frequency. It is now recognised as a regular occurrence in double-blind clinical trials of new medicines, where people taking an inert, placebo control pill develop side-effects of the drug being tested – if they are warned about them beforehand. If the nocebo effect can make us ill in clinical trials, it can also do so in real life. Placebo and Nocebo effects Rubin, researcher at King’s College London, has been following up the early Californian experiments to investigate the role of the nocebo effect in a debilitating condition that afflicts increasing numbers of westerners, including, reportedly, one in every 20 Swedes. Electrosensitivity is supposedly caused by exposure to the electromagnetic fields given off by power lines, mobile phone handsets and masts, wi-fi and other electrical appliances. Sufferers report a range of symptoms, including headaches, tingling skin, dry throat, eye irritations, problems with memory and concentration, dizziness, inflammation and muscle and joint pains. Crucially, Rubin’s research provides little reason to think that the reported symptoms are caused by the electromagnetic fields. “There is no robust evidence that there is a direct link, and there is reasonably robust evidence that there is no link,” he says. Experiments show that electrosensitive people are no better than anybody else at identifying when they are being exposed to an electric field. And when they are exposed, sufferers report more symptoms only if they have been told that they are being exposed. It is this sort of evidence that has led to the failure of court cases brought against mobile phone companies by people who believe their symptoms to be caused by their handsets. Rubin has little doubt that the symptoms exhibited by electrosensitives are real. “Something cognitive is going on – something to do with the expectation is triggering the symptoms,” he says. And the idea of a psychological basis to electrosensitivity is supported by the fact that cognitive behavioural therapy can be an effective treatment. Even non-specific worries about the state of the world in general can be enough to make us ill, according to research from the University of Auckland. Prior to a huge pesticide-spraying programme in New Zealand, people were asked about their attitudes towards new technologies and environmental threats. After the spraying, those who were most concerned about the health risks reported the greatest number of symptoms. If negative expectations generate symptoms, what generates the negative expectations? , professor emeritus at University College London and an expert on risk, says there is a current fashion among health professionals and other authorities for issuing precautionary advice concerning the health risks posed by new medicines and technologies. And that, he says, is a major contributing factor. Despite finding a dearth of evidence for a risk to health from mobile phones, The Report, published in 2000 by the Independent Expert Group on Mobile Telephones and Health (and named after the group’s chairman, Sir ), recommended that people limit their use of the technology, just in case. says that such an ambiguous message is an example of just the sort of thing that can encourage people to keep an eye out for signs of illness. A similar message accompanied the UK Health Protection Agency’s 2004 report on the dangers of exposure to electromagnetic fields. “The report found no evidence of an aetiological connection between the fields and the symptoms,” says . “And yet at the press conference that launched it, they went beyond that and said that people who believe they are sensitive should stay away from electromagnetic radiation.” Hence the ensuing headline from the Daily Mail: “Household appliances such as kettles, computers and microwaves could be health hazards, a government expert suggested yesterday.” The Swedish government has issued similarly precautionary advice. “This process is more general than just taking a pill,” says Arthur Barsky, professor of psychiatry at Harvard Medical School. “However you induce negative expectations – whether it’s through a pill, a health warning or advertising by a drug company – you set in motion the same process of symptom reattribution, which then amplifies the symptom, which further solidifies the reattribution.” Wainwright, a sociologist at the University of Bath, agrees that health warnings issued for precautionary reasons can be bad for our health: “It’s a bit like flicking through a medical encyclopaedia and imagining you’ve got symptoms of disease X, Y and Z. But this effect has a lot more potency when it’s a health professional telling you this stuff.” Not even health professionals are immune. Medical students, who flick through more than their fair share of medical encyclopaedias, are themselves susceptible to a phenomenon known as “medical students’ disease”, whereby they develop symptoms of whatever condition they happen to be studying. Research has shown that medics display more signs of hypochondria than their peers studying law. Today, the “worried well” are more likely to go online than to a library to self-diagnose, in which case patient websites such as Medinfo or NetDoctor could be exacerbating symptoms at the same time as raising awareness of them. No research has yet been conducted that directly addresses internet health sites and the nocebo effect. But research published last year by psychologists at Harvard and Yale universities raises the possibility that even our weight is susceptible to the power of suggestion. The study looked at the health of hotel chambermaids over a four-week period. Half of subjects were told at the start that their work counted as good exercise that satisfied the recommendations of the US surgeon-general. A month later, they had lost weight and gained other health benefits such as lower blood pressure. Their uninformed colleagues showed no change. This puzzling finding does not seem to be explained by extra exercise prompted by the briefing: this was weight-loss by placebo effect. And where there is a placebo effect, nocebo is often lurking in the shadows. In an age when health professionals and governments are inclined to talk of obesity as an epidemic, of the entire population as unhealthy and to demand that we exercise more, such pronouncements might be doing more harm than good. “I very much believe that health warnings can be adversely effective via a nocebo effect,” says Alia Crum, one of the chambermaid study’s authors. “Whether or not this overrides the benefit of getting the message out there is another story.” Her current research attempts to tease apart these conflicting effects. Not all health warnings are likely to cause what they are intended to prevent. Warnings on cigarette packets don’t give anyone cancer, for example; in fact, no rigorous research has turned up evidence that cancer, or any other serious, fatal disease, can be caused or cured by the power of negative or positive thinking – although both might well influence the strength of the symptoms. But it is not the cancer statistics that many of these scientists fret over. Their concern is the rise of conditions such as back pain, fibromyalgia (chronic fatigue syndrome) and food allergies, which are characterised by the symptoms expressed rather than by the underlying biology. These symptoms may have a biological basis in some individuals and a psychological one in others. So, while rare and tragic deaths resulting from extreme food allergies can be attributed to physiological anaphylactic shock, health warnings on food that “may contain nuts” might be contributing to the rise of food allergies in general, and to the statistics that less than 20 per cent of teenagers who believe they have a food allergy actually test positive. “It’s quite remarkable how the illnesses that are increasing at the moment are not the big, killer diseases, grounded in real, physical pathology,” says Wainwright. “It’s the subjective problems of everyday life that are becoming medicalised, where there’s no actual evidence of any physical illness. “Even hassles at work are interpreted through the medicalised category of work stress rather than political or industrial relations issues” – a practice that Wainwright says is promoted by government agencies. “Health policy is promoting this belief that we’re all at risk from absolutely everything we come into contact with, and that just encourages us to feel more vulnerable and to interpret our normal experiences as health problems. It’s all just amplifying this epidemic of non-specific illness, which has incredibly disabling effects on people.” Certain changes in doctors’ bedside manner might have been counter-productive. “Doctors feel more and more bound to warn patients about any possible consequences of taking a drug,” says Arthur Barsky of Harvard, which can mean reeling off a long list of possible, albeit unlikely side-effects. “There’s clearly a fundamental reason for putting out health information,” he says, “but I think it’s become a kind of hysteria.” If that hysteria means that more patients stop taking a medicine, having been put off by possible side-effects, then both the effectiveness of medical treatments and people’s confidence in conventional medicine is undermined. It is one reason, says Barsky, that people are putting increasing faith in alternative therapies. And a loss of confidence in conventional medicine and its practitioners might itself induce a nocebo effect that detracts further from its efficacy. We are actually freer from serious illness than our grandparents were, and yet we worry about our health more than they did. Barsky calls this the “paradox of health”: dramatic improvements in our clinical well-being, accompanied by a rise in dissatisfaction with our health and a tendency to report more symptoms. He suspects that excessive awareness-raising is at least partly to blame. at UCL has coined a phrase for the overemphasis on possible negative outcomes by health professionals and their political masters: compulsive risk assessment psychosis, or more succinctly, Crap. The phenomenon has emerged, he says, from a climate of low and declining trust, in which the health professions practise defensively. “The doctors aren’t trusting their patients not to sue them. There’s all this ass-covering going on all over the place.” But if they don’t look out, health professionals and policymakers might find that this strategy has left them dangerously exposed to attack from other directions. Legal cases brought against mobile phone companies by people who believe that the electromagnetic radiation given off by their handsets is making them ill might have failed, but cases brought against those who issue health warnings, on the grounds that it exacerbates illness through the nocebo effect, might prove more successful. At least they would have some scientific evidence to support their claims. Stuart Blackman is a freelance writer with a focus on science and the environment Copyright The Financial Times Limited 2009 " FT " and " Financial Times " are trademarks of the Financial Times. Privacy policy | Terms © Copyright The Financial Times Ltd 2009. McAfee Music Director/Conductor Triangle Youth Ballet Adjunct Instructor of Horn, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill Former Principal Horn, North Carolina Symphony (1992-2007) 919.787.3022 Quote Link to comment Share on other sites More sharing options...
Recommended Posts
Join the conversation
You are posting as a guest. If you have an account, sign in now to post with your account.
Note: Your post will require moderator approval before it will be visible.