Guest guest Posted December 22, 2006 Report Share Posted December 22, 2006 I did not mean to be gruff or scare anyone....... I am sorry if I did that........Thank you Pam for this article...... merry merry happy happy and don't stress, again I apologize if I caused Kitty or any of you any undo worry. hugs, sally Why We Worry About The Things We Shouldn't... ...And Ignore The Things > We Should > By JEFFREY KLUGER > > Posted Sunday, Nov. 26, 2006 > Correction Appended: November 27, 2006 > > It would be a lot easier to enjoy your life if there weren't so many > things trying to kill you every day. The problems start even before > you're fully awake. There's the fall out of bed that kills 600 Americans > each year. There's the early-morning heart attack, which is 40% more > common than those that strike later in the day. There's the fatal plunge > down the stairs, the bite of sausage that gets lodged in your throat, > the tumble on the slippery sidewalk as you leave the house, the > high-speed automotive pinball game that is your daily commute. > > Other dangers stalk you all day long. Will a cabbie's brakes fail when > you're in the crosswalk? Will you have a violent reaction to bad food? > And what about the risks you carry with you all your life? The father > and grandfather who died of coronaries in their 50s probably passed the > same cardiac weakness on to you. The tendency to take chances on the > highway that has twice landed you in traffic court could just as easily > land you in the morgue. > > Shadowed by peril as we are, you would think we'd get pretty good at > distinguishing the risks likeliest to do us in from the ones that are > statistical long shots. But you would be wrong. We agonize over avian > flu, which to date has killed precisely no one in the U.S., but have to > be cajoled into getting vaccinated for the common flu, which contributes > to the deaths of 36,000 Americans each year. We wring our hands over the > mad cow pathogen that might be (but almost certainly isn't) in our > hamburger and worry far less about the cholesterol that contributes to > the heart disease that kills 700,000 of us annually. > > We pride ourselves on being the only species that understands the > concept of risk, yet we have a confounding habit of worrying about mere > possibilities while ignoring probabilities, building barricades against > perceived dangers while leaving ourselves exposed to real ones. Six > Muslims traveling from a religious conference were thrown off a plane > last week in Minneapolis, Minn., even as unscreened cargo continues to > stream into ports on both coasts. Shoppers still look askance at a bag > of spinach for fear of E. coli bacteria while filling their carts with > fat-sodden French fries and salt-crusted nachos. We put filters on > faucets, install air ionizers in our homes and lather ourselves with > antibacterial soap. " We used to measure contaminants down to the parts > per million, " says Dan McGinn, a former Capitol Hill staff member and > now a private risk consultant. " Now it's parts per billion. " > > At the same time, 20% of all adults still smoke; nearly 20% of drivers > and more than 30% of backseat passengers don't use seat belts; > two-thirds of us are overweight or obese. We dash across the street > against the light and build our homes in hurricane-prone areas--and when > they're demolished by a storm, we rebuild in the same spot. Sensible > calculation of real-world risks is a multidimensional math problem that > sometimes seems entirely beyond us. And while it may be true that it's > something we'll never do exceptionally well, it's almost certainly > something we can learn to do better. > > AN OLD BRAIN IN A NEW WORLD > > Part of the problem we have with evaluating risk, scientists say, is > that we're moving through the modern world with what is, in many > respects, a prehistoric brain. We may think we've grown accustomed to > living in a predator-free environment in which most of the dangers of > the wild have been driven away or fenced off, but our central nervous > system--evolving at a glacial pace--hasn't got the message. > > To probe the risk-assessment mechanisms of the human mind, ph > LeDoux, a professor of neuroscience at New York University and the > author of The Emotional Brain, studies fear pathways in laboratory > animals. He explains that the jumpiest part of the brain--of mouse and > man--is the amygdala, a primitive, almond-shaped clump of tissue that > sits just above the brainstem. When you spot potential danger--a stick > in the grass that may be a snake, a shadow around a corner that could be > a mugger--it's the amygdala that reacts the most dramatically, > triggering the fight-or-flight reaction that pumps adrenaline and other > hormones into your bloodstream. > > It's not until a fraction of a second later that the higher regions of > the brain get the signal and begin to sort out whether the danger is > real. But that fraction of a second causes us to experience the fear far > more vividly than we do the rational response--an advantage that doesn't > disappear with time. The brain is wired in such a way that nerve signals > travel more readily from the amygdala to the upper regions than from the > upper regions back down. Setting off your internal alarm is quite easy, > but shutting it down takes some doing. > > " There are two systems for analyzing risk: an automatic, intuitive > system and a more thoughtful analysis, " says Slovic, professor of > psychology at the University of Oregon. " Our perception of risk lives > largely in our feelings, so most of the time we're operating on system > No. 1. " > > There's clearly an evolutionary advantage to this natural timorousness. > If we're mindful of real dangers and flee when they arise, we're more > likely to live long enough to pass on our genes. But evolutionary > rewards also come to those who stand and fight, those willing to take > risks--and even suffer injury--in pursuit of prey or a mate. Our > ancestors hunted mastodons and stampeded buffalo, risking getting > trampled for the possible payoff of meat and pelt. Males advertised > their reproductive fitness by fighting other males, willingly engaging > in a contest that could mean death for one and offspring for the other. > > These two impulses--to engage danger or run from it--are constantly at > war and have left us with a well-tuned ability to evaluate the costs and > payoffs of short-term risk, say Slovic and others. That, however, is not > the kind we tend to face in contemporary society, where threats don't > necessarily spring from behind a bush. They're much more likely to come > to us in the form of rumors or news broadcasts or an escalation of the > federal terrorism-threat level from orange to red. It's when the risk > and the consequences of our response unfold more slowly, experts say, > that our analytic system kicks in. This gives us plenty of opportunity > to overthink--or underthink--the problem, and this is where we start to > bollix things up. > > WHY WE GUESS WRONG > > Which risks get excessive attention and which get overlooked depends on > a hierarchy of factors. Perhaps the most important is dread. For most > creatures, all death is created pretty much equal. Whether you're eaten > by a lion or drowned in a river, your time on the savanna is over. > That's not the way humans see things. The more pain or suffering > something causes, the more we tend to fear it; the cleaner or at least > quicker the death, the less it troubles us. " We dread anything that > poses a greater risk for cancer more than the things that injure us in a > traditional way, like an auto crash, " says Slovic. " That's the dread > factor. " In other words, the more we dread, the more anxious we get, and > the more anxious we get, the less precisely we calculate the odds of the > thing actually happening. " It's called probability neglect, " says Cass > Sunstein, a University of Chicago professor of law specializing in risk > regulation. > > The same is true for, say, AIDS, which takes you slowly, compared with a > heart attack, which can kill you in seconds, despite the fact that heart > disease claims nearly 50 times as many Americans than AIDS each year. We > also dread catastrophic risks, those that cause the deaths of a lot of > people in a single stroke, as opposed to those that kill in a chronic, > distributed way. " Terrorism lends itself to excessive reactions because > it's vivid and there's an available incident, " says Sunstein. " Compare > that to climate change, which is gradual and abstract. " > > Unfamiliar threats are similarly scarier than familiar ones. The next E. > coli outbreak is unlikely to shake you up as much as the previous one, > and any that follow will trouble you even less. In some respects, this > is a good thing, particularly if the initial reaction was excessive. But > it's also unavoidable given our tendency to habituate to any unpleasant > stimulus, from pain and sorrow to a persistent car alarm. > > The problem with habituation is that it can also lead us to go to the > other extreme, worrying not too much but too little. Sept. 11 and > Hurricane Katrina brought calls to build impregnable walls against such > tragedies ever occurring again. But despite the vows, both New Orleans > and the nation's security apparatus remain dangerously leaky. " People > call these crises wake-up calls, " says Dr. Irwin Redlener, associate > dean of the Mailman School of Public Health at Columbia University and > director of the National Center for Disaster Preparedness. " But they're > more like snooze alarms. We get agitated for a while, and then we don't > follow through. " > > THE COMFORT OF CONTROL > > We similarly misjudge risk if we feel we have some control over it, even > if it's an illusory sense. The decision to drive instead of fly is the > most commonly cited example, probably because it's such a good one. > Behind the wheel, we're in charge; in the passenger seat of a crowded > airline, we might as well be cargo. So white-knuckle flyers routinely > choose the car, heedless of the fact that at most a few hundred people > die in U.S. commercial airline crashes in a year, compared with 44,000 > killed in motor-vehicle wrecks. The most white-knuckle time of all was > post--Sept. 11, when even confident flyers took to the roads. Not > surprisingly, from October through December 2001 there were 1,000 more > highway fatalities than in the same period the year before, in part > because there were simply more cars around. " It was called the '9/11 > effect.' It produced a third again as many fatalities as the terrorist > attacks, " says Ropeik, an independent risk consultant and a former > annual instructor at the Harvard School of Public Health. > > Then too there's what Ropeik and others call " optimism bias, " the thing > that makes us glower when we see someone driving erratically while > talking on a cell phone, even if we've done the very same thing, perhaps > on the very same day. We tell ourselves we're different, because our > call was shorter or our business was urgent or we were able to pay > attention to the road even as we talked. What optimism bias comes down > to, however, is the convenient belief that risks that apply to other > people don't apply to us. > > Finally, and for many of us irresistibly, there's the irrational way we > react to risky behavior that also confers some benefit. It would be a > lot easier to acknowledge the perils of smoking cigarettes or eating too > much ice cream if they weren't such pleasures. Drinking too much confers > certain benefits too, as do risky sex, recreational drugs and uncounted > other indulgences. This is especially true since, in most cases, the > gratification is immediate and the penalty, if it comes at all, comes > later. With enough time and enough temptation, we can talk ourselves > into ignoring almost any long-term costs. " These things are fun or hip, > even if they can be lethal, " says Ropeik. " And that pleasure is a > benefit we weigh. " > > If these reactions are true for all of us--and they are--then you might > think that all of us would react to risk in the same way. But that's > clearly not the case. Some people enjoy roller coasters; others won't go > near them. Some skydive; others can't imagine it. Not only are thrill > seekers not put off by risk, but they're drawn to it, seduced by the > mortal frisson that would leave many of us cold. " There's an internal > thermostat that seems to control this, " says risk expert of > University College London. " That set point varies from person to person > and circumstance to circumstance. " > > No one knows how such a set point gets calibrated, but evidence suggests > that it is a mix of genetic and environmental variables. In a study at > the University of Delaware in 2000, researchers used personality surveys > to evaluate the risk-taking behavior of 260 college students and > correlated it with existing research on the brain and blood chemistry of > people with thrill-seeking personalities or certain emotional disorders. > Their findings support the estimate that about 40% of the high-thrill > temperament is learned and 60% inherited, with telltale differences in > such relevant brain chemicals as serotonin, which helps inhibit > impulsive behavior and may be in short supply in people with high-wire > personalities. > > CAN WE DO BETTER? > > Given these idiosyncratic reactions, is it possible to have a rational > response to risk? If we can't agree on whether something is dangerous or > not or, if it is, whether it's a risk worth taking, how can we come up > with policies that keep all of us reasonably safe? > > One way to start would to be to look at the numbers. Anyone can agree > that a 1-in-1 million risk is better than 1 in 10, and 1 in 10 is better > than 50-50. But things are almost always more complicated than that, a > fact that corporations, politicians and other folks with agendas to push > often deftly exploit. > > Take the lure of the comforting percentage. In one study, Slovic found > that people were more likely to approve of airline safety-equipment > purchases if they were told that it could " potentially save 98% of 150 > people " than if they were told it could " potentially save 150 people. " > On its face this reaction makes no sense, since 98% of 150 people is > only 147. But there was something about the specificity of the number > that the respondents found appealing. " Experts tend to use very > analytic, mathematical tools to calculate risk, " Slovic says. " The > public tends to go more on their feelings. " > > There's also the art of the flawed comparison. Officials are fond of > reassuring the public that they run a greater risk from, for example, > drowning in the bathtub, which kills 320 Americans a year, than from a > new peril like mad cow disease, which has so far killed no one in the > U.S. That's pretty reassuring--and very misleading. The fact is that > anyone over 6 and under 80--which is to say, the overwhelming majority > of the U.S. population--faces almost no risk of perishing in the tub. > For most of us, the apples of drowning and the oranges of mad cow > disease don't line up in any useful way. > > But such statistical straw men get trotted out all the time. People > defending the safety of pesticides and other toxins often argue that you > stand a greater risk of being hit by a falling airplane (about 1 in > 250,000 over the course of your entire life) than you do of being harmed > by this or that contaminant. If you live near an airport, however, the > risk of getting beaned is about 1 in 10,000. Two very different > probabilities are being conflated into one flawed forecast. " My favorite > is the one that says you stand a greater risk from dying while skydiving > than you do from some pesticide, " says Egan Keane of the Natural > Resources Defense Council. " Well, I don't skydive, so my risk is zero. " > > Risk figures can be twisted in more disastrous ways too. Last year's > political best seller, The One Percent Doctrine, by journalist Ron > Suskind, pleased or enraged you, depending on how you felt about war in > Iraq, but it hit risk analysts where they live. The title of the book is > drawn from a White House determination that if the risk of a terrorist > attack in the U.S. was even 1%, it would be treated as if it were a 100% > certainty. Critics of Administration policy argue that that 1% > possibility was never properly balanced against the 100% certainty of > the tens of thousands of casualties that would accompany a war. That's a > position that may be easier to take in 2006, with Baghdad in flames and > the war grinding on, but it's still true that a 1% danger that something > will happen is the same as a 99% likelihood that it won't. > > REAL AND PERCEIVED RISK > > It's not impossible for us to become sharper risk handicappers. For one > thing, we can take the time to learn more about the real odds. Baruch > Fischhoff, professor of social and decision sciences at Carnegie Mellon > University, recently asked a panel of 20 communications and finance > experts what they thought the likelihood of human-to-human transmission > of avian flu would be in the next three years. They put the figure at > 60%. He then asked a panel of 20 medical experts the same question. > Their answer: 10%. " There's reason to be critical of experts, " Fischhoff > says, " but not to replace their judgment with laypeople's opinions. " > > The government must also play a role in this, finding ways to frame > warnings so that people understand them. Graham, formerly the > administrator of the federal Office of Information and Regulatory > Affairs, says risk analysts suffer no end of headaches trying to get > Americans to understand that while nuclear power plants do pose dangers, > the more imminent peril to both people and the planet comes from the > toxins produced by coal-fired plants. Similarly, pollutants in fish can > be dangerous, but for most people--with the possible exception of small > children and women of childbearing age--the cardiac benefits of fish > easily outweigh the risks. " If you can get people to compare, " he says, > " then you're in a situation where you can get them to make reasoned > choices. " > > Just as important is to remember to pay proper mind to the dangers that, > as the risk experts put it, are hiding in plain sight. Most people no > longer doubt that global warming is happening, yet we live and work in > air-conditioned buildings and drive gas-guzzling cars. Most people would > be far likelier to participate in a protest at a nuclear power plant > than at a tobacco company, but it's smoking, not nukes, that kills an > average of 1,200 Americans every single day. > > We can do better, however, and leaders in government and industry can > help. The residual parts of our primitive brains may not give us any > choice beyond fighting or fleeing. But the higher reasoning we've > developed over millions of years gives us far greater--and far more > nuanced--options. Officials who provide hard, honest numbers and a > citizenry that takes the time to understand them would not only mean a > smarter nation, but a safer one. [This article contains a complex > diagram. Please see hardcopy or pdf.] TOTAL ANNUAL DEATHS 2.5 MILLION > Homicide 17,732 Suicide 31,484 > > Terrified of bees, snakes and swimming pools? ACCIDENTS 109,277 Maybe > you should worry more about your heart DISEASES 2.3 million Other > diseases 681,150 Diabetes 74,219 Chronic lower-respiratory disease > 126,382 Stroke 157,689 Cancer 556,902 Heart disease 685,089 All other > deaths 8,364 Sources: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; > National Transportation Safety Board > > Correction: The original version of this story incorrectly identified > Ropeik as a " former professor at the Harvard School of Public > Health. " In fact, Mr. Ropeik was a former annual instructor, not a > professor, and he was not a member of the school's faculty. > > > With reporting by Bjerklie/New York, Dan Cray/Los Angeles > > http://www.time.com/time/magazine/article/0,9171,1562978,00.html > > " and the beat goes on....... " Sonny Bono --------------Learn the art of patience. Apply discipline to your thoughts when theybecome anxious over the outcome of a goal. Impatience breeds anxiety,fear, discouragement and failure. Patience creates confidence,decisiveness, and a rational outlook, which eventually leads tosuccess. -- __________________________________________________ Quote Link to comment Share on other sites More sharing options...
Recommended Posts
Join the conversation
You are posting as a guest. If you have an account, sign in now to post with your account.
Note: Your post will require moderator approval before it will be visible.